EurekaAlert:
he research builds on a long series of marshmallow-related studies
that began at Stanford University in the late 1960s. Walter Mischel and
other researchers famously showed that individual differences in the
ability to delay gratification on this simple task correlated strongly
with success in later life. Longer wait times as a child were linked
years later to higher SAT scores, less substance abuse, and parental
reports of better social skills.
Because of the surprising correlation, the landmark marshmallow
studies have been cited as evidence that qualities like self-control or
emotional intelligence in general may be more important to navigating
life successfully than more traditional measures of intelligence, such
as IQ.
The Rochester team wanted to explore more closely why some
preschoolers are able to resist the marshmallow while others succumb to
licking, nibbling, and eventually swallowing the sugary treat. The
researchers assigned 28 three- to five-year-olds to two contrasting
environments: unreliable and reliable. The study results were so strong
that a larger sample group was not required to ensure statistical
accuracy and other factors, like the influence of hunger, were accounted
for by randomly assigning participants to the two groups, according to
the researchers. In both groups the children were given a
create-your-own-cup kit and asked to decorate the blank paper that would
be inserted in the cup.
In the unreliable condition, the children were provided a
container of used crayons and told that if they could wait, the
researcher would return shortly with a bigger and better set of new art
supplies for their project. After two and a half minutes, the research
returned with this explanation: "I'm sorry, but I made a mistake. We
don't have any other art supplies after all. But why don't you use these
instead?" She then helped to open the crayon container.
Next a quarter-inch sticker was placed on the table and the
child was told that if he or she could wait, the researcher would return
with a large selection of better stickers to use. After the same wait,
the researcher again returned empty handed.
The reliable group experienced the same set up, but the
researcher returned with the promised materials: first with a rotating
tray full of art supplies and the next time with five to seven large,
die-cut stickers.
The marshmallow task followed, with the explanation that the child
could have "one marshmallow right now. Or – if you can wait for me to
get more marshmallows from the other room – you can have two
marshmallows to eat instead." The researcher removed the art supplies
and placed a single marshmallow in a small desert dish four inches from
the table's edge directly in front of the child. From an adjoining room,
the researchers and the parent observed through a computer video camera
until the first taste or 15 minutes had lapsed, whichever came first.
All children then received three additional marshmallows.
"Watching their strategies for waiting was quite entertaining,"
says Holly Palmeri, coauthor and coordinator of the Rochester Baby Lab.
Kids danced in their seats, sang, and took pretend naps. Several took a
bite from the bottom of the marshmallow then placed it back in the
desert cup so it looked untouched. A few then nibbled off the top,
forgetting they could then longer hide the evidence since both ends were
eaten, she said.
"We had one little boy who grabbed the marshmallow immediately and we
thought he was going to eat it," recalled Kidd. Instead he sat on it.
"Instead of covering his eyes, he covered the marshmallow."
Children who experienced unreliable interactions with an
experimenter waited for a mean time of three minutes and two seconds on
the subsequent marshmallow task, while youngsters who experienced
reliable interactions held out for 12 minutes and two seconds. Only one
of the 14 children in the unreliable group waited the full 15 minutes,
compared to nine children in the reliable condition.
"I was astounded that the effect was so large," says Aslin. "I
thought that we might get a difference of maybe a minute or so… You
don't see effects like this very often."
In prior research, children's wait time averaged between 6.08 and
5.71 minutes, the authors report. By comparison, manipulating the
environment doubled wait times in the reliable condition and halved the
time in the unreliable scenario. Previous studies that explored the
effect of teaching children waiting strategies showed smaller effects,
the authors report. Hiding the treat from view boosted wait times by
3.75 minutes, while encouraging children to think about the larger
reward added 2.53 minutes.
I've always been a delayed gratification person, and have been amazed at how quickly some folks are able to spend money when they come into it. But this study makes pretty damn good sense. Honestly, I've always been in a stable environment where waiting for rewards works out. Other folks have never been so fortunate. Besides that, I've always known that whatever comes about, I can buy what I want if I decide to. Again, other folks haven't been so lucky. It is great that somebody threw a twist into the study and came up with a very interesting result. It reminds me of
this study in some ways. In it, people who thought they were lucky were willing to take shortcuts, while those who thought they were unlucky didn't. I though it would have been interesting if the shortcut was the wrong answer in that study, just to see how the lucky people felt when they got screwed.
No comments:
Post a Comment